科研成果

2023
元尺考. 故宫博物院院刊 [Internet]. 2023. 访问链接
马啸、马佳磊、严洁. 公民因何遵从:政策执行中的价值认同与社会规范. 新视野 [Internet]. 2023. 知网链接Abstract
公民是否主动遵守公共政策对国家治理具有重要意义。国家如何以较小代价获得公民在日常社会治理政策领域的政策遵从?从激励与压力两个维度出发,可将公民遵守日常社会政策的行为划分为“不服从”“价值认同服从”“强制性服从”与“社会规范性服从”四种类型。在城市垃圾分类新规实施后,对北京市城镇居民履行垃圾分类义务的问卷调查发现:价值认同与社会规范是实现公民政策服从的关键因素,并且二者发挥了同样重要的影响。相比之下,因当前法规惩戒性有限,强制性服从尚未成为影响公民履行垃圾分类义务的关键因素。本文的发现对在社会治理领域运用“助推型”政策干预策略具有借鉴意义。
魏一璞. 利用视觉情境范式揭示口语加工的时间进程 [Visual world paradigm reveals the time course of spoken language processing]. 心理科学进展 [Advances in Psychological Science] [Internet]. 2023;31(11):2050-2062. Full textAbstract
The visual world paradigm (VWP) is a widely used tool in psycholinguistics to study the time course of spoken language processing (Cooper, 1974; Tanenhaus et al., 1995). In this paradigm, eye movements are tracked while participants listen to spoken language and view visual scenes, providing precise temporal information about the processing of words and sentences. As acoustic input unfolds, comprehenders’ focus of attention on particular entities in the mental representations of spoken language changes, and their visual attention also shifts accordingly (Altmann & Mirkovi, 2009). Such allocation of attention can be manifested in eye movements as overt behavioral data. Linking hypotheses of this field link eye movements in visual contexts with the mental representations of linguistic input. The coordinated interplay account proposed by Knoeferle and Crocker (2006, 2007) defines three phases in visually situated spoken language comprehension: integrating new words, searching for referents in visual contexts and matching linguistic input with objects and actions in the visual contexts. These three phases may take place sequentially or overlap with one another in time. An alternative linking hypothesis raised by Altmann and Mirkovi (2009), however, suggested that the processes of interpreting linguistic input and comprehending visual scenes are intertwined, as linguistic meanings and non-linguistic information (e.g., visual information and world knowledge) are stored in one unitary system and jointly contribute to the dynamic representation of situations. Salverda et al.’s (2011) goal-based linking hypothesis introduces a task-goal dimension into the theoretical model. That is, the goal of the task also affects language processing: Visual objects that are directly related to this goal would attract more attention; and additional tasks such as clicking or moving objects contribute to the goal structure of the task and directly influence eye movements. The assets visual world paradigm has brought to the field—(i) the possibility to include a visual dimension in linguistic processing; (ii) a fine-grained time course measure of eye movements in real-time language comprehension—have greatly expanded the range of experimental designs for language studies. As the VWP relies primarily on listening tasks and does not require subjects to have full literacy skills in reading, it can be applied to examine language processing in young children, second language learners, and people with specific language impairments. Dependent variables in a VWP experiment include fixation proportions, target ratio, latency of saccades, etc. Factors such as areas of interest, groups and experimental conditions can be included as independent variables. To make use of the fine-grained time-course data provided by the VWP, including a temporal dimension to the analytical models is crucial. While traditional analyses evaluate fixation/saccade differences between conditions during a time window (using t-test, ANOVA, and mixed-effect models), the divergent point analysis and cluster‑based permutation analysis are informative in detecting and comparing the emergence time of effects (Ito & Knoeferle, 2022). The growth-curve analysis, on the other hand, models the changes of looks to an interest area over time (Mirman, 2008). Studies fueled by the VWP have revealed that language processing is incremental or even predictive, in contrast to the findings of earlier studies supporting delayed integration of language. At the early stage of word recognition, phonological cohorts compete with the target, and listeners may use phonetic information to anticipate upcoming words. The processing of semantic information in verb-argument and classifier-noun structures, for example, is highly incremental or anticipatory. Discourse processing, including referential processing and the comprehension of coherence relations, is also found to be immediate. In addition, the VWP has shown that the syntactic and pragmatic processing is in accordance with the constraint-based account (Trueswell et al., 1994)—multiple types of information including syntactic structures and pragmatic implicatures, form constraints to language processing at the very early stage, alongside other constraints such as contextual features, visual information, world knowledge, etc. The VWP is limited in the sense that it cannot provide data on processing time and therefore cannot answer questions related to processing difficulties in language comprehension. Moreover, the VWP experiments can only present a limited number of static objects in visual space, which also differs from the complex visual environment of natural conversation. In experimental settings where only a limited number of objects are presented, listeners may anticipate linguistic input in advance and look strategically at certain objects (Henderson & Ferreira, 2004; see counter-argument in Dahan & Tanenhaus, 2004). Developments of the VWP are driven by both theoretical and technological advances. For future studies, investigating the role of task-goal in real-time language processing situated in visual contexts is important. Technological innovations such as virtual reality (VR) create comparatively natural communication scenarios while maintaining precise experimental control, largely improving the ecological validity of eye-tracking experiments using the VWP.
张忞煜. 印度中学历史教科书的“去莫卧儿”风波. 世界知识. 2023;(13):62-64.
张忞煜. 去殖民化,印度总理莫迪的“政治软饮”. 世界知识. 2023;(1):65-67.
陈昂轩, 刘怀亚 贾积有 张君 徐玄冲. 双重身份学生自适应学习模型的构建与实证研究. 现代教育技术. 2023;33(10):108-117.
余典、马佳磊、吴小希. 发展政治学代表学者. In: 《发展政治学学科地图》. 北京: 北京大学出版社; 2023. pp. 437-448. 豆瓣链接Abstract
发展政治学作为一门现代社会科学学科,其兴起和发展离不开关注政治发展的学者们的共同努力。本章介绍十五位代表性学者,这些选择是兼顾了平衡性的结果。值得关注的是,发展政治学的代表学者中不仅有政治学者,也不乏经济学者、社会学者和历史学者。政治学以外的相关学科领域与政治学的互动与交融共同推进了发展政治学研究。
马佳磊、余典、张逸凡. 发展政治学经典著作. In: 《发展政治学学科地图》. 北京: 北京大学出版社; 2023. pp. 449-455. 豆瓣链接Abstract
对发展政治学这样一门尚在不断发展中的学科而言,其研究深度和广度的拓展离不开对核心概念、经典议题及研究范式的把握。前辈学者在开创和探索的过程中已然渐次铺展开一幅发展政治学的“学科地图”,而作为其思想凝练的产物, 经典著作为发展政治学的研究者提供了参考和超越的“地标”。有鉴于此,在兼顾和平衡学科内不同研究问题、方法和范式的基础上,本章介绍在发展政治学领域具有代表性的经典著作。
Wu C-Y. 古罗马的立法说服文化:婚姻法制化的案例研究 [Legislative Persuasion Culture in Ancient Rome: A Case Study of Legalization of Marriage Laws], in 北京大学—首都师范大学 世界史青年学者学术交流会[Academic Exchange Conference for Young Scholars in World History between Peking University and Capital Normal University]. 首都师范大学 Capital Normal University; 2023.Abstract
奥古斯都三度受元老院与人民任命,出任法律道德监管(curator legum et morum, 19 BCE, 18 BCE, 11 BCE, cf. RGDA 6, Dio Cass. 54.30.1, Suet. Aug. 27),推动婚姻立法,塑造新罗马社会,但多有面对社会面不满的压力(RGDA 8; Eck 2022)。本文先考虑奥古斯都时期婚姻立法过程中,持不同立场的人士所使用的说服手段,以诗人贺拉斯和普罗佩提乌斯为代表。再来,本文看迪奥卡西乌斯笔下的奥古斯都面对骑士序列人士的两段演说(Dio Cass. 56.1-10),虽然较贺拉斯和普罗佩提乌斯而言,成文的时间距奥古斯都时期较远,但应有一定的参考价值。最后,本文也考虑一般学界不列入考虑的铭刻材料。公元前17年的《世纪竞赛元老院决议文》(Senatus Consultum de ludibus saecularibus)以及公元8年前后的《涂丽雅悼词》(Laudatio Turiae)等铭文石刻,或有助于我们理解不同层次的反应与反制。铭文勒石不只是一种昭示性质的行为,也是一种表演性质的行为。这两方在婚姻立法过程中立起的石刻,与婚姻、生育、单身、无子等议题相关,应该要与婚姻立法问题更为紧密地考虑。本文主张,这两方石刻可以理解为支持与反对婚姻立法人士要试图说明自己立场、并说服不同立场者的公开尝试,反应的是奥古斯都为道德监管的大环境下,罗马公民仍然留存的共识文化。
吴仲达, 梁婧涵, 陆昌勤. 可持续职业生涯:概念、管理策略与研究展望. 外国经济与管理 [Internet]. 2023;45(6). 访问链接Abstract
在职业环境不断变化且充满不确定性的时代,实现职业生涯的可持续发展对于 组织和员工个体都十分重要的。因此,可持续职业生涯的话题应运而生,并成为职业生涯领域 前沿性的理论构念。国外学者已对此进行了较深入的探讨与研究,但尚未引起国内学者的充分 关注,且缺乏系统的整理。鉴于此,本文旨在对可持续职业生涯这一新概念的内涵以及研究进 展进行系统的评述,并阐述其在不确定职业生涯环境下对组织与个体职业生涯管理的意义。首 先,我们介绍了可持续职业生涯这一概念的提出背景、内涵界定、理论解读和影响因素;然后, 进一步从个体、组织,以及非工作领域三个方面阐述了如何实现可持续职业生涯;最后,评述了 可持续职业生涯的最新研究进展,并提出了六个值得深入探讨的未来研究方向。本文旨在为理 解可持续职业生涯的研究及进展提供整体性的框架,并为不确定环境下的职业生涯管理实践 提供有价值的建议。
刘智锋;王继民. 可解释机器学习在信息资源管理领域的应用研究综述. 数据分析与知识发现. 2023.
唐震怡, 张鹏翼. 团队成员态度分化程度对知识构建过程的影响研究. 图书情报工作. 2023;67(11):77.
董申, 马莉萍. 国际比较视角下的通识教育实施体系研究. 河北师范大学学报(教育科学版) [Internet]. 2023;25(01):71-78. 访问链接Abstract
对美国、日本、新加坡、英国、德国、法国等发达国家的通识教育体系开展比较研究,可将其大致分为显性和隐性两类,各国对通识教育的认知历史和实施体系有较大差异,通识教育的重心早则在中学即已结束,迟则贯穿大学阶段,这折射出通识教育仍存在较大的复杂性和争议性。中国的通识教育在历史上受到较多外部影响,目前还在发展变迁之中。从各国经验来看,有必要将通识教育的研究范畴从高等教育扩展到中等教育,对通识教育的机制设计、实施阶段和课程安排也应将大学和中学综合考虑。在飞速发展的环境下,通识教育理念应在教育体制中提早树立,统筹实施,助力学生全面发展。建立科学、可比的通识教育评价方法对通识教育的设计和改进有重大意义。
谢小荣, 陈垒 马宁宁 等.; 2023. 基于多分量 Morlet 小波的高频振荡相量测量方法及装置. China patent CN ZL 202111109397.4.
贾积有, 乐惠骁 张誉月 刘怀亚 陈昂. 基于大数据挖掘的智能评测和辅导系统设计. 中国电化教育. 2023;2023(3):112-119.
苏志国, 陈伟东, 郑宇涵, 危婕, 李菲菲, 陈嘉瑜, 陈吕军, 温东辉. 基于宏基因组学解析不同污水处理系统的耐药基因组分布特征和传播机制. 生态毒理学报 [Internet]. 2023;18(2):1-13. 访问链接Abstract
污水处理厂是向水环境中传播抗生素抗性基因(antibiotic resistance genes, ARGs)的热点。与城镇污水相比,工业园区废水成分复杂、污染物浓度高,更有利于ARGs的增殖和扩散。为探究不同类型废水环境的ARGs组成特征和潜在的传播风险,采用宏基因组学技术分别对城镇生活污水处理系统(W1-SD)、工业园区废水处理系统(W1-SI)和2个城镇综合污水处理系统(W2-LH1和W2-LH2)进行取样调查。结果显示,多重耐药类、磺胺类、氨基糖苷类和杆菌肽类抗性基因是废水环境中的主要耐药类型,Ⅰ型整合子、转座酶基因等可移动遗传元件(MGEs)对sul1、aadA和ereA等基因亚型的增殖扩散发挥了关键作用,通过序列分型发现质粒型ARGs的相对丰度更高,尤其是在进水样品中,氨基糖苷类和磺胺类等抗性基因是主要的质粒型ARGs;污水处理过程削减了ARGs多样性,且经过二次沉淀工艺,ARGs丰度均明显降低,但在W1-SI和W2-LH2中,后续的深度处理工艺又使ARGs丰度升高;与城镇污水处理系统相比,W1-SI的ARGs组成更为稳定,最终排水中富集了较高丰度的质粒型ARGs,同时识别到了高频率的潜在水平基因转移事件和2条携带多种抗性基因的重叠群序列(contigs),表明工业园区废水排放具有更高的ARGs传播风险。本研究丰富了不同类型废水环境耐药基因组的已有认知,为有效管控废水排放特别是工业园区废水排放的健康风险提供了科学依据。
盛曼玉, 王俊, 朱茜茜, 张鹏翼. 基于扎根理论的数字反哺机制探析——以子代的视角. 图书情报知识. 2023;40(4):52-61.
《小微企业经营者和个体工商户信用评价指南 (T/NIFA 17-2023)》. [Internet]. 2023. 访问链接
谢侃侃. 局外人的“局内观”:韩素音笔下的马来亚“紧急状态”. In: 区域. Vol. 10. 北京: 社会科学文献出版社; 2023. pp. 81-95.
陈腾. 《广装潢志》的四部稿本. 文献. 2023;(4):178-189.

Pages